References

Adams RJ, Stocks NP, Wilson DH Health literacy – a new concept for general practice?. Australian Family Physician.. 2009; 38:(3)144-147

Ahmed S, Bryant LD, Cole P Midwives' perceptions of their role as facilitators of informed choice in antenatal screening. Midwifery.. 2013; 29:(7)745-750 https://doi.org/10.1016/j.midw.2012.07.006

Andersson E, Christensson K, Hildingsson I Mothers' satisfaction with group antenatal care versus individual antenatal care – a clinical trial. Sexual and Reproductive Healthcare.. 2013; 4:(3)113-120 https://doi.org/10.1016/j.srhc.2013.08.002

Beukeboom CJ, Tanis M, Vermeulen IE The language of extraversion: extraverted people talk more abstractly, introverts are more concrete. Journal of Language and Social Psychology.. 2013; 32:(2)191-201 https://doi.org/10.1177/0261927X12460844

Beukeboom CJ Mechanisms of linguistic bias: How words reflect and maintain stereotypic expectancies. In: Laszlo J, Forgas J., Vincze O New York: Psychology Press; 2014

Beulen L, van den Berg M, Faas BHW The effect of a decision aid on informed decision-making in the era of non-invasive prenatal testing: a randomised controlled trial. European Journal of Human Genetics.. 2016; 24:(10)1409-1416 https://doi.org/10.1038/ejhg.2016.39

Bilker WB, Hansen JA, Brensinger CM Development of abbreviated nine-item forms of the Raven's standard progressive matrices test. Assessment.. 2012; 19:(3)354-369 https://doi.org/10.1177/1073191112446655

Burke S., Bennett C., Bedward J., Farndon P The experiences and preferences of people receiving genetic information from healthcare professionals. Birmingham: NHS National Genetics Education and Development Centre.. 2007; 1-43

Bryant LD, Green JM, Hewison J The role of attitudes towards the targets of behaviour in predicting and informing prenatal testing choices. Psychology and Health.. 2010; 25:(10)1175-1194 https://doi.org/10.1080/08870440903055893

Cacioppo JT, Petty RE The need for cognition. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology.. 1982; 42:(1)116-131 https://doi.org/10.1037/0022-3514.42.1.116

Cacioppo JT, Petty RE, Feng Kao C The efficient assessment of need for cognition. Journal of Personality Assessment.. 1984; 48:(3)306-307 https://doi.org/10.1207/s15327752jpa4803_13

Cacioppo JT, Petty RE, Feinstein JA, Jarvis WBG Dispositional differences in cognitive motivation: the life and times of individuals varying in need for cognition. Psychological Bulletin.. 1996; 119:(2)197-253 https://doi.org/10.1037/0033-2909.119.2.197

Campbell WH, Ritchie AJ Proverb interpretation in forensic evaluations. American Academy of Psychiatry and the Law.. 2002; 27:24-27

New Jersey: Centre for Health Care Strategies; 2013

Coaley KLondon: Sage; 2009

Cohen AR, Stotland E, Wolfe DM An experimental investigation of need for cognition. J Abnorm Psychol.. 1955; 51:(2)291-294

Cole M Cognitive development and formal schooling: The evidence from cross-cultural research. In: Moll LC Cambridge: Cambridge University Press; 1990

Deane-Gray T Effective communication. In: Peate I, Hamilton C Chicester, West Sussex: John Wiley and Sons; 2008

Deery R, Fisher P ‘Switching and swapping faces’: performativity and emotion in midwifery. International Journal of Work Organisation and Emotion.. 2010; 3:(3)270-286 https://doi.org/10.1504/IJWOE.2010.032926

de Jong A, Dondorp WJ, Macville MVE Microarrays as a diagnostic tool in prenatal screening strategies: ethical reflection. Human Genetics.. 2014; 133:(2)163-172 https://doi.org/10.1007/s00439-013-1365-5

Dormandy E, Michie S, Hooper R, Marteau TM Low uptake of prenatal screening for Down syndrome in minority ethnic groups and socially deprived groups: a reflection of women's attitudes or a failure to facilitate informed choices?. International Journal of Epidemiology.. 2005; 34:(2)346-52 https://doi.org/10.1093/ije/dyi021

Dormandy E, Michie S, Hooper R, Marteau TM Informed choice in antenatal Down syndrome screening: a cluster-randomised trial of combined versus separate visit testing. Patient Education and Counselling.. 2006; 61:(1)56-64 https://doi.org/10.1016/j.pec.2005.02.006

Dowswell T, Carroli G, Duley L Alternative versus standard packages of antenatal care for low-risk pregnancy. Cochrane Database of Systematic Reviews.. 2010; 6:(10) https://doi.org/10.1002/14651858.CD000934.pub2

Erby LH, Roter D, Larson S, Cho J The rapid estimate of adult literacy in genetics (REAL-G): a means to assess literacy deficits in the context of genetics. American Journal of Medical Genetics Part A.. 2008; 146A:(2)174-181 https://doi.org/10.1002/ajmg.a.32068

Fitzpatrick R, Hopkins ALondon: Royal College of Physicians of London; 1993

Flesch R A new readability yardstick. J Appl Psychol.. 1948; 32:(3)221-33 https://doi.org/10.1037/h0057532

Flesch JR, Kincaid CBoston: Houghton Mifflin; 1965

Gekas J, Langlois S, Ravitsky V Non-invasive prenatal testing for fetal chromosome abnormalities: review of clinical and ethical issues. The Application of Clinical Genetics.. 2016; 9:15-26 https://doi.org/10.2147/TACG.S85361

Gorham DR A proverbs test for clinical and experimental use. Psychological Reports.. 1956; 2:(1)1-12

Haines HM, Hildingsson I, Pallant JF, Rubertsson C The role of women's attitudinal profiles in satisfaction with the quality of their antenatal and intrapartum care. Journal of Obstetric, Gynecology and Neonatal Nursing.. 2013; 42:(4)428-441 https://doi.org/10.1111/1552-6909.12221

Heim AWLondon: Oxford University Press; 1975

Hunter B The importance of reciprocity in relationships between community-based midwives and mothers. Midwifery.. 2006; 22:(4)308-322 https://doi.org/10.1016/j.midw.2005.11.002

John S: University of South Wales; 2017

Jones S, 2nd edn.. Petersfield, Hampshire: Harriman House; 2010

Keating D Adolescent thinking. In: Adelson J New York: Wiley; 1979

Kim KS Interviewing: beginning to see each other, 2nd edn.. In: Uhlmann WR, Scuhette JL, Yashar B New Jersey: Wiley-Blackwell; 2009

Koenke K Readability formula use and misuse. Read Teach.. 1987; 40:672-674

Leap N, Hunter BLondon: Scarlet Press; 1993

Lehman DR, Nisbett RE A longitudinal study of the effects of undergraduate training on reasoning. Developmental Psychology.. 1990; 26:(6)952-960 https://doi.org/10.1037/0012-1649.26.6.952

Ley P Cognitive variables and non-compliance. J Compliance Health Care.. 1986; 1:171-188

Marshalek B, Lohman DF, Snow RE The complexity continuum in the radex and hierarchical models of intelligence. Intelligence.. 1983; 7:(2)107-127 https://doi.org/10.1016/0160-2896(83)90023-5

Michie S, Marteau T Genetic counselling: some issues of theory and practice. In: Marteau T, Richards M Cambridge: Cambridge University Press; 1996

Nelson HEWindsor: NFER-Nelson; 1982

Norton ME, Jacobsson B, Swamy GK, Laurent LC, Ranzini AC, Brar H, Tomlinson MW, Pereira L, Spitz JL, Hollemon D, Cuckle H, Musci TJ, Wapner RJ Cell-free DNA analysis for non invasive examination of trisomy. The New England Journal of Medicine.. 2015; 372:1589-1597

Ormond KE From genetic counseling to “genomic counseling”. Molecular Genetics and Genomic Medicine.. 2013; 1:(4)189-193 https://doi.org/10.1002/mgg3.45

Paradice RDinton, Wiltshire: Quay Books; 2002

Parry R, Stuart-Hamilton I Animism begins at forty: evidence that animism and other naïve beliefs are established before the onset of old age. Educational Gerontology.. 2010; 36:(10–11)1043-1050 https://doi.org/10.1080/03601271003756594

Piaget J Intellectual evolution from adolescence to adulthood. Human Development.. 1972; 15:(1)1-12 https://doi.org/10.1159/000271225

Pope R, Cooney M, Graham L, Holliday M, Patel S Aspects of care 6: continuing educational needs of midwives 2. British Journal of Midwifery.. 1998; 6:(5)298-302 https://doi.org/10.12968/bjom.1998.6.5.298

Porter S, Crozier K, Sinclair M, Kernohan WG New midwifery? A qualitative analysis of midwives' decision-making strategies. Journal of Advanced Nursing.. 2007; 60:(5)525-34 https://doi.org/10.1111/j.1365-2648.2007.04449.x

Raven JCLondon: HK Lewis; 1962

Roter DL, Erby LH, Larson S, Ellington L Assessing oral literacy demand in genetic counseling dialogue: preliminary test of a conceptual framework. Social Science and Medicine.. 2007; 65:(7)1442-1457 https://doi.org/10.1016/j.socscimed.2007.05.033

Roter DL, Erby L, Larson S, Ellington L Oral literacy demand of prenatal genetic counseling dialogue: predictors of learning. Patient Education and Counseling.. 2009; 75:(3)392-7 https://doi.org/10.1016/j.pec.2009.01.005

Sadoski M, Kealy WA, Goetz ET, Paivio A Concreteness and imagery effects in the written composition of definitions. Journal of Educational Psychology.. 1997; 89:(3)518-526 https://doi.org/10.1037/0022-0663.89.3.518

Semin GR, Fiedler K The cognitive functions of linguistic categories in describing persons: social cognition and language. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology.. 1988; 54:(4)558-568 https://doi.org/10.1037/0022-3514.54.4.558

Skirton H, Barr O Antenatal screening and informed choice: a cross-sectional survey of parents and professionals. Midwifery.. 2010; 26:(6)596-602 https://doi.org/10.1016/j.midw.2009.01.002

Sofaer S, Firminger K Patient perceptions of the quality of health services. Annual Review of Public Health.. 2005; 26:(1)513-559 https://doi.org/10.1146/annurev.publhealth.25.050503.153958

Stern RA, Prohaska ML Neuropsychological evaluation of executive functioning. In: Dickstein LJ, Oldham JM, Riba MB 1996

Stuart-Hamilton I, McDonald L Do we need intelligence? Some reflections on the importance of “g”. Educational Gerontology.. 2001; 27:(5)399-407 https://doi.org/10.1080/03601270152053429

Tait AR, Voepel-Lewis T, Malviya S Factors that influence parents' assessments of the risks and benefits of research involving their children. Pediatrics.. 2004; 113:(4)727-32 https://doi.org/10.1542/peds.113.4.727

Tennant M, 3rd edn.. Abingdon, Oxfordshire: Routledge; 2005

Thompson S, Noblin SJ, Lemons J, Peterson SJ, Carreno C, Harbison A perceptions of Latinas on the traditional prenatal genetic counseling model. Journal of Genetic Counseling.. 2015; 24:(4)675-82 https://doi.org/10.1007/s10897-014-9797-1

Exeter: UK National Screening Committee; 2016 https://legacyscreening.phe.org.uk/policydb_download.php?doc=602

Wechsler D, Coalson DL, Raiford SESan Antonio, TX: Pearson; 2008

Weil JNew York: Oxford University Press; 2000

A new tool to assess understanding of Down syndrome screening information presented by midwives

02 November 2019
Volume 27 · Issue 11

Abstract

Aim

To identify factors that could influence women's understanding of Down syndrome (DS) screening information presented by midwives.

Methods

Current literature was scrutinised. Components that could influence women's understanding were identified and a new framework was developed and refined. Measures were selected and developed to create a tool to assess the framework.

Findings

A new framework and assessment tool, measuring understanding of screening information and communication (MUSIC), was developed to assess women's understanding of DS screening information, their cognitive status and the midwives' communicative style.

Conclusion

This framework is the first of its kind, encompassing both women's cognitive status and midwife communication as an influence on women's understanding. Applying the framework and tool could inform midwifery practice by providing an insight into whether, to what extent and how, cognitive status influences understanding of DS screening information, the importance of tailoring information to each woman and highlighting areas of communication that are most effective.

In England, Wales and Scotland, all pregnant women are routinely offered antenatal screening for Down syndrome (DS) at the booking appointment, according to the UK National Screening Committee ([UKNSC], 2007). Screening information should increase knowledge in order for women to make informed decisions (de Jong et al, 2014). However, some women undergo screening even though they have relatively little knowledge of the test or the condition (Dormandy et al, 2006; Skirton and Barr, 2010).

Advances in genetic/genomic technology are revolutionising antenatal screening and the UK NSC, (2016) has recommended introducing non-invasive prenatal testing (NIPT) into the National Health Service (NHS) which is more accurate than current forms of screening (Norton et al, 2015). The way healthcare professionals present information is likely to influence screening uptake. Midwives should counsel women about NIPT in the same way as invasive testing because the tests carry similar diagnostic implications. If midwives can effectively communicate current screening information and support women's informed decision making, then it is anticipated that it will be easier to incorporate NIPT into practice.

This paper proposes a framework to investigate what factors influence women's understanding of DS screening information. The two key factors in this are midwife communication and women's cognitive status.

Midwife communication

A number of researchers have attempted to describe components of language which could influence a listener's understanding. Adams et al (2009) suggest healthcare professionals should use plain language, limit medical jargon, use diagrams to aid verbal explanation and check clients have understood information.

Roter et al (2009) found that individuals with low literacy learnt more in prenatal genetic counselling sessions which were more interactive, had fewer dense chunks of speech and shorter speeches from genetic counsellors. In contrast, individuals with high literacy benefited from more complex language, dense chunks of speech and less interactivity. The differences required for optimum learning in those with low and high literacy skills highlight the importance of tailoring information, and this has been echoed by a number of researchers (Paradice, 2002; Ormond, 2013).

In order to tailor information, it is important that women's current knowledge is established early in the appointment, otherwise time may be wasted either describing DS, when women already have full knowledge of the condition, or assuming knowledge and describing screening when women have no knowledge of DS (Bryant et al, 2010). The Nursing and Midwifery Council (2018) states that midwives should ‘check people's understanding’. However, midwives have said that they often do not ask open questions and encourage time-consuming interactive conversation due to workload (Porter et al, 2007). Ongoing assessments of understanding at appointments are essential to facilitate informed decision making (Dormandy et al, 2005).

Individuals with low literacy skills may be disadvantaged when presented with written or oral dialogue and are less likely to understand medical information regarding risks and benefits (Tait et al, 2004; Erby et al, 2008; Roter et al, 2009). Information presented in diagram or picture form to support oral explanations may aid understanding, especially in those with low literacy (Centre for Health Care Strategies, 2013).

Oral information may be difficult to understand due to its subjective nature. Words can be either abstract or concrete. Concrete words allow the formation of images in our minds (Sadoski et al, 1997). For instance, it is easy to conjure an image of the concrete words ‘chair’ or ‘needle’. It is much harder to produce an image in our mind of abstract words, such as ‘care’ or ‘risk’.

Genetic risk information is often abstract, which can complicate communication and influence whether information is understood and remembered by the recipient (Kim, 2009; Beukeboom et al, 2013). Roter et al (2009) found that individuals with low literacy skills had superior learning when information was more concrete. Arguably, due to the subjective nature of abstract words, it would be preferable to communicate using only concrete words to ensure understanding. However, this seems to not be the case, as Roter et al (2009) demonstrates that individuals with higher literacy learnt better in prenatal genetic counselling sessions when more abstract information was presented.

The role that abstract-concrete language plays in women's understanding has not previously been explored in relation to DS screening in the UK. Further research is necessary to help clarify whether concrete language aids understanding for all individuals, or whether tailoring language, as abstract or concrete, enhances understanding.

Cognitive status

The term cognitive status encompasses a whole set of mental processes such as attention, memory, intelligence, problem solving and reasoning. Cognitive status plays a role in how people process the world around them. Therefore, it is proposed that it may influence how women understand DS screening information.

Cognitive development occurs in stages and the ability to reason abstractly emerges at the age of 12 years (Piaget, 1972). Abstract reasoning forms the basis of logic and allows individuals to apply knowledge to novel situations and correctly solve problems by imagining alternative solutions (Stern and Prohaska, 1996; Campbell and Ritchie, 2002; Tennant, 2005). However, the speed of development can vary from one individual to another, and it has been suggested that some adults may never truly gain abstract reasoning (Piaget, 1972; Keating, 1979; Cole, 1990; Lehman and Nisbett, 1990).

Some individuals are more likely to ask questions and be more active in discussions than others. These individuals are said to possess high Need for Cognition (NfC). NfC is the extent that people engage in thinking to increase their knowledge (Cohen et al, 1955). Levels of NfC may be an indicator of understanding. For instance, women with high NfC have an ‘information seeker’ disposition, meaning they are more likely to ask questions requiring the midwife to provide more information. Thus, NfC could influence both women's understanding and midwife communication.

Cognitive status, in terms of abstract-concrete reasoning (Piaget, 1972) and NfC (Cacioppo and Petty, 1982), has not previously been investigated as an influence on how women understand DS screening information or the midwife's communicative style.

Satisfaction

Dissatisfaction with healthcare often results from a lack of communication (Roter et al, 2007; Deane-Gray, 2008). Good communication should result in women gaining an improved understanding of screening information and a greater satisfaction with the services received (Pope et al, 1998; Paradice, 2002).

In summary, there is a lack of literature on the influence of abstract language, women's cognitive status and resources on women's understanding of DS screening information. There is little evidence outlining how midwives check women's knowledge and understanding, and whether they tailor information to suit each woman. All these factors have driven the creation of a new framework and tool to identify factors that may influence women's understanding of screening information.

Methods

Aims

The aim was to investigate what factors influence women's understanding of DS screening information. The study was split into two distinct phases:

  • Phase 1 involved developing a framework and measures to address the research questions. This is the focus of the current paper
  • Phase 2 involved applying and testing the framework in practice. This will be discussed in subsequent papers.
  • Literature review

    An extensive literature review was undertaken across multiple databases. This identified different factors that could influence women's understanding of DS screening information. These components can be broken down into aspects of midwife communication and women's cognitive status. These are discussed further in subsequent sections and are incorporated into the new framework.

    Developing the framework

    Phase 1 involved developing a new framework, measuring understanding of screening information and communication (MUSIC), as a tool to assess women's understanding of DS screening information, their cognitive status and midwives' communication. The following aspects were considered during the development of the tool:

  • It should be applicable to booking appointments when screening discussions occur
  • It should assess the interactive nature of communication between the midwife and woman
  • It should break down communication into multiple components which could influence women's understanding
  • It should assess the relevance of women's cognitive status
  • It should assess the primary outcome: women's understanding
  • It should assess the secondary outcome: women's satisfaction
  • It should provide an objective measure to ensure inter-rater reliability and test-retest reliability.
  • MUSIC and midwife communication

    Koenke (1987) outlined the following factors which could influence understanding of written information: syntactic complexity, concept density, abstractness, organisation, coherence, sequence of ideas, page format, length of line print, length of paragraph, punctuation, illustrations, colour and reader interest.

    Key components to ensure women's understanding include checking knowledge, understanding and explaining medical terms, inviting questions, and using diagrams (Ley, 1986). This research uses similar components to assess midwife's oral communication.

    Language complexity

    Koenke's ‘syntactic complexity’ is mirrored in the language complexity component of MUSIC (Koenke, 1987). As highlighted by Ley (1986), the importance of explaining medical terms is vital since medical terminology may be unknown. Words such as ‘inheritance’ and ‘susceptibility’ are unfamiliar to the general population (Erby et al, 2008) and the term ‘genetics' itself has different meanings to different people (Burke et al, 2007).

    Dynamics

    Depending on the dynamics of conversation, information may be easier or harder for women to understand and process. If midwives provide screening information in a dense chunk or ‘lesson type’ format, there may be inadequate time for the woman to process all the information provided (Roter et al, 2009). More interactive speech allows equal contribution from the woman and midwife (Hunter, 2006; Deery and Fisher, 2010).

    Knowledge and understanding check

    Women's pre-existing knowledge and perception of genetics may affect their comprehension and recall of provided information (Michie and Marteau, 1996; Thompson et al, 2015). Questioning clients' knowledge can help guide the remainder of the appointment, providing the healthcare professional with insight into areas that are not fully understood and require further explanation (Weil, 2000). Questioning women's understanding throughout the appointment can clarify any misunderstandings and ensure the woman is making an informed decision. However, there are currently no guidelines to outline exactly how midwives should check women's understanding (Ahmed et al, 2013).

    Resources

    The resources component of MUSIC encompasses Koenke's (1987) idea of including illustrations and colour in written materials, and Ley's (1986) use of diagrams to aid understanding. The extent that resources are used to aid midwives' verbal explanations is unknown.

    Abstract language

    The abstract language component endorses Koenke's (1987) idea of ‘abstractness'. The research may illustrate whether concrete language aids understanding for all individuals, or whether tailoring language, as abstract or concrete, enhances understanding.

    Originally, additional components of ‘non-directive’, ‘length of appointment’ and ‘technical terminology’ were considered for MUSIC. These were subsequently excluded for a number of reasons. Non-directive communication does not influence understanding, but rather decision-making—which was not assessed in this research. Length of DS discussion was measured instead of length of appointment, since only the section of the appointment that covered DS screening information was analysed and not the whole appointment. Technical terminology makes language more complex, therefore assessment of technical terminology has been included in the ‘language complexity’ component of MUSIC.

    MUSIC and women's cognitive status

    The exploration of cognitive status could highlight the role it plays, not only in women's understanding, but also in influencing the communication between the midwife and woman. NfC can influence women's understanding and midwife communication, since the ‘information seeker’ disposition of those with high NfC means they might ask more questions and the midwife will provide more information. The best predictor of success on Piagetian tasks, and thus demonstration of abstract reasoning skills, is NfC (Stuart-Hamilton and McDonald, 2001; Parry and Stuart-Hamilton, 2010). Therefore, it is expected that women with higher abstract reasoning skills should have higher NfC.

    Satisfaction

    Ensuring a positive pregnancy experience goes beyond ensuring the physical health of the woman and her infant (Haines et al, 2013). It is important that women's satisfaction with antenatal care is established to gain their views regarding how and whether communication can be improved.

    Findings

    A new framework, MUSIC, was developed to provide an insight into influences on women's understanding of DS screening information provided in antenatal appointments (Figure 1).

    Figure 1. The MUSIC framework with communication, cognition and outcome measures. Cognitive measures are within dotted lines as these may be mediating factors, rather than direct influences, on understanding

    Developing and selecting measures to assess midwife communication

    This research has involved developing new measures and combining existing measures to create a tool to assess the framework (Table 1).


    Language complexity Number: word count and sentencesAverage: sentences per paragraph, words per sentencesReadability: passive sentences, Flesch reading ease and Flesch-Kincaid Grade Level. Passive sentence measures how informative text is; the higher the score the more complex and formal the text. The higher the score on the Flesch reading ease, the easier the text is to understand:
  • Score    Difficulty
  • 0–40    Very difficult to difficult
  • 40–80    Average
  • 80+    Easy to very easy
  • Flesch-Kincaid Grade Level should aim for a score of 4.0–5.0Technical terminology score: if any of the following eight words were used in the appointment it was noted whether the midwife provides an explanation of these words or not: diagnostic, amniocentesis, amniotic, screening, chromosome, abnormalities, millilitres, obstetrician
    Dynamics Interactivity: number of speaking turns in a session per minutePace: average number of syllables per word × total transcript word count/session length (in seconds)Duration: average duration in seconds spanning a block of uninterrupted speech
    Check knowledge/understanding Knowledge check: Do midwives check women's current knowledge levels when they commence the appointment?Understanding check: Do midwives check that women understand the information throughout the appointment? How do midwives check understanding: do they explicitly ask or use paraphrasing?
    Resources Are additional resources used to aid explanations, eg pictograms?
    Abstract language The linguistic category model (LCM) (Semin and Fiedler, 1988): the higher the score, the more abstract the text. Four word categories are distinguished to produce an ‘abstract score’, computed as follows:
  • Word Type                            Score
  • Descriptive action verbs (eg yell, hit, walk)                1
  • Interpretative action verbs and State action verbs (eg help)         2
  • State verbs (eg think, admire, hate, appreciate)              3
  • Adjectives (eg social, aggressive, honest, reliable)             4
  • Scoring matrix for these measures is set out in Appendix 1 (John, 2017)

    Developing and selecting measures for assessment

    Two questionnaires were designed to assess:

  • Women's demographics, cognitive status and their understanding of DS screening information found in Appendix 2 (John, 2017).
  • Women's satisfaction with DS screening information provided by midwives found in Appendix 3 (John, 2017).
  • The measures used to assess these concepts are discussed. Appendix 4 (John, 2017) contains a detailed scoring system for the questionnaires.

    Cognitive status

    For the purposes of this research, cognitive ability will be assessed on the abstract-concrete continuum. Abstract tests correlate highly with, and draw on, more components of intelligence than concrete tests, which correlate less with, and draw on, fewer components of intelligence (Marshalek et al, 1983). The tests that have been selected vary by reasoning level (concrete versus abstract), domain (verbal versus non-verbal), and difficulty, allowing a comprehensive assessment of women's concrete-abstract reasoning.

    Cognitive ability: verbal abstract reasoning measure

    Gorham's proverbs (1956) have been used widely within psychology to classify individuals who do not possess abstract thought. Proverbs measure verbal reasoning and can assess where individuals lie on the abstract-concrete continuum (Campbell and Ritchie, 2002). Participants will be provided with multiple-choice response proverbs. This ensures better standardisation of the measure as each answer is scored as abstract (2), somewhat abstract (1) or concrete (0). Open questions could elicit many responses which would take longer to code and cause difficulty in defining the answer on the abstract-concrete continuum. This study does not rely on proverbs alone as an assessment of abstract ability. Raven's standard progressive matrices (RSPM) is also used to measure non-verbal reasoning ability.

    Cognitive ability: non-verbal abstract reasoning measure)

    The original RSPM consists of 60 items with five sets containing 12 items each. Each set represents a different conceptual theme and increases in difficulty, therefore, each set requires a different thought process (Jones, 2010). Each item within RSPM requires the identification of relationships between patterns and reasoning to make comparisons between them (Coaley, 2009). Due to time constraints and attrition concerns, a shorter nine-item version will be employed, which has the same predictive power, reliability and validity as the 60 item matrices (Bilker et al, 2012). The nine items (A11, B12, C4, C12, D7, D12, E1, E5, E7) sample each conceptual theme of the full matrices.

    Alternate tests of cognitive ability were considered. The Mill Hill vocabulary scale and the national adult reading test were disregarded because they do not measure abstract reasoning ability but verbal intelligence instead, and neither of these scales would capture women's ability to solve novel problems, such as those presented by the midwife (Raven, 1962; Nelson, 1982). The Alice Heim and the Wechsler adult intelligence scale (WAIS-IV) were also considered (Heim, 1975; Wechsler et al, 2008). However, time was a concern due to the length of these tests. Furthermore, WAIS-IV assesses intelligence as a whole, however the current research aims to capture only abstract-concrete reasoning, therefore, the majority of the scale would be disregarded. While WAIS-IV includes proverbs as an assessment of verbal reasoning abilities and uses pictures to assess non-verbal reasoning abilities, the current study selected shorter tests.

    Need for cognition

    The NfC scale was developed by Cacioppo et al (1984; 1996), who tested the scale on different populations and reported a reliability coefficient of a=0.90. The test includes 18 statements where individuals score on a Likert scale the extent that they enjoy thinking about particular tasks and exerting cognitive effort.

    DS understanding

    A six item multiple-choice understanding questionnaire was developed to assess understanding of DS information covered in the booking appointment. Questions were created from similar studies which included questionnaires to assess understanding of screening information and from information in the Antenatal Screening Wales leaflet (2013).

    Satisfaction measures

    Care must be taken with the interpretation of satisfaction questionnaires since participants often do not want to criticise their healthcare provider and thus generally provide high satisfaction ratings (Fitzpatrick and Hopkins, 1993; Dowswell et al, 2010; Andersson et al, 2013). However, when service users are asked more specific questions about aspects of their healthcare, they tend to be more critical (Sofaer and Firminger, 2005). Thus, specific questions regarding certain aspects of information provided by the midwife are included. The satisfaction questionnaire will be sent to women a week after their appointment and thus prior to receiving any screening results, which could influence their satisfaction with provided information.

    Demographics

    A demographics section will capture women's age, ethnicity, English language ability and parity. Features of the appointment which could affect communication will be captured, such as the presence of another during the appointment, or the appointment setting, home or clinic.

    Piloting

    The questionnaire was piloted with a convenience sample of colleagues and lay individuals (n=45). Based on the results of the pilot, it was anticipated that the questionnaire to assess women's cognitive status and DS understanding would take approximately 20 minutes to complete, and the satisfaction questionnaire would take approximately 5 minutes. Time was an important consideration when designing the questionnaires to try and create as short a questionnaire as possible without loss of functionality.

    Discussion

    Previous research has established that not all women are fully informed regarding DS screening (Dormandy et al, 2006; Beulen et al, 2016). Due to the introduction of NIPT, there will be additional pre-screening information for women to understand within booking appointments (Department of Health, 2016). It is imperative that DS screening information is currently understood before a test with greater implications is fully introduced.

    This paper has introduced phase 1 of a study which involved developing a novel framework and tool to assess factors which could influence women's understanding of DS screening information. While some factors have previously been considered, such as language complexity and dynamics (Roter et al, 2009), MUSIC is the first of its kind to encompass a combination of factors which could influence women's understanding. All or some components of the framework may influence women's understanding of DS screening information.

    In phase 2, the research team will apply the MUSIC tool to assess women's understanding of DS screening information, their cognitive status and midwives' communicative style. The study that will follow will encompass a mixed-methods design with two distinct components, transcript analysis of recorded consultations and quantitative questionnaires.

    This research is the first to simultaneously evaluate multiple aspects of midwife communication and women's cognitive status as an influence on their understanding of DS screening information. By revealing the dayto-day consultations between midwives and women, insight into the way DS screening is communicated and consequently understood can be obtained. The framework may clarify the importance of tailoring information to women's cognitive status by gaining an insight into how it influences women's understanding and midwife communication.

    Conclusion

    During phase 1, a framework, MUSIC, has been developed. This incorporates a combination of factors which could influence women's understanding of screening information. Once the tool has been tested, the results will advance current knowledge in this field, both in terms of aspects of midwife communication that are effective in facilitating informed choice as well as outlining the role that cognitive status plays in women's understanding of screening information. It is anticipated that findings will be assembled to inform a best practice model for midwifery. The scope of the framework means that any recommendations proposed may have relevance to information provision beyond midwife communication of DS screening.

    Key points

  • A new framework, was developed to assess understanding of screening information
  • The framework encompasses midwife communication and women's cognitive status
  • The findings could inform a best practice model for midwifery practice
  • The tool developed may be applicable beyond Down syndrome (DS) screening information